Table of Contents |
Original Article
| ||||||
Transfer of learning of a pattern drawing task in typically developing children | ||||||
Kinsuk Maitra1, Martin S. Rice2, Hae Yean Park3 | ||||||
1PhD, OTR/L, Chair and Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, Florida International University, Miami, FL. USA.
2PhD, OTR/L, FAOTA, Professor and Program Director, Department of Rehabilitation Sciences, University of Toledo, Toledo, OH. USA. 3PhD, OT, Assistant Professor, Department of Occupational Therapy, Yonsei University, Wonju-si, South Korea. | ||||||
| ||||||
[HTML Abstract]
[PDF Full Text]
[Print This Article]
[Similar article in Pumed] [Similar article in Google Scholar] |
How to cite this article |
Maitra K, Rice MS, Park HY. Transfer of learning of a pattern drawing task in typically developing children Edorium J Disabil Rehabil 2016;2:1–8. |
Abstract
|
Aims:
The purpose of this study was to explore whether typically developing children learn a motor skill better if the skill is first learned by the proximal motor apparatus and then transferred to the distal motor apparatus.
Methods: Thirty-one typically developing children were randomly assigned to either proximal or distal muscle group. The children in the proximal muscle group learned to draw a large pattern using the proximal muscle groups (shoulder and elbow) and transferred the skill to the distal muscle group (wrist and hand) by reproducing a similar but smaller pattern. The children in the distal muscle group did the opposite. A two-way ANOVA is used for data analysis. Results: The results showed that the interaction between the main factor of direction and condition yielded significance. Also, skill was transferred in both in proximal to distal (P2D) and distal to proximal (D2P) children group. Error units were decreased between original learning (OL) and transfer learning (TL) in P2D children group. Conclusion: The results further suggest that children would be able to learn a novel motor task using either their proximal motor apparatus that is shoulder and elbow or using their distal motor apparatus that is wrist and hands. Future study should focus on indicating if it would help for child with intellectual disabilities or motor impairments to learn drawing tasks. | |
Keywords:
Children, Drawing, Handwriting, Intervention, Transfer of learning
|
Introduction
| ||||||
In a pediatric setting, occupational therapists work routinely with fine motor problems in children. Examples include addressing problems with managing buttons, zippers, shoelaces, self-care, tool use, leisure occupations, and most importantly handwriting. Therapists use a number of strategies including a range of graded fine motor activities to promote in-hand manipulation and functional performance in children [1] [2] . Some of the examples to address poor hand skills are: a) picking up small objects such as cereal and M&M's with tweezers; b) pinching a ziplock bag while using each finger with opposition of the thumb; c) using the ulnar digits to hold a small tube of toothpaste while twisting open the cap using the thumb, index, and middle finger; d) transferring a key or other small objects from the palm to the fingertips of one hand; and e) rolling small balls of clay, Silly Putty, or Play Dough between the tip of the thumb and tips of the index and middle finger [3]. While occupational therapists frequently use the fine motor apparatus to improve fine motor skill, motor control and motor learning, researchers have found that skill acquisition is not limb specific rather goal specific. In other words, a skill that has been acquired by one limb is not restricted to that limb [4][5]. Past researchers have shown that practice of a novel motor task with one hand facilitates the performance of the same task of the unpracticed opposite hand [6]. This limb-to-limb relationship is known as intermanual transfer, lateral transfer of motor learning, or cross education [5] . Nagel and Rice (2000) investigated intermanual transfer in their study where subjects were randomly assigned to a training or control group. In the training group, subjects completed a toy maze with either their right or left hand. In the control group, the subjects did not practice the maze. Results revealed a significant decrease in movement time and force oscillations in the untrained limb of the trained group, indicating a transfer advantage of the skill to the untrained limb when the contralateral limb acquires a skill [7]. In addition, Andree and Maitra (2002) investigated intermanual transfer with a writing occupation in young adults without disabilities. The subjects learned to write a foreign alphabet letter with either their left or right hand. Later in the study the subjects were asked to write the letter with the unpracticed hand.The researchers discovered a transfer advantage of the task between hands when movement time and movement size was measured and the transfer did not depend on the hand dominance [5] . On the other hand, the transfer of a motor skill from either the proximal to the distal effector group or vice versa is called intramanual transfer. Vangheluwe et al. (2004) reported intramanual transfer when participants learned a new task of drawing (star-line drawing paradigm) [8]. Participants performed two different tasks with the right and left limb in parallel. Thirty-six undergraduate students were assigned to two different groups. In the first group the participants practiced the star-line drawing task with the proximal motor apparatus using the shoulder and elbow joints, and then transferred the performance to the distal motor apparatus using the wrist and finger joints. In the second group the participants practiced the star-line drawing task with the wrist-finger joints (distal) and then transferred the performance to the shoulder-elbow joints (proximal). The participants practiced the star-line task for 25 trials per day for three days. The results showed that there was a transfer advantage of the skill of line performance (error of orientation angle) from the proximal to the distal motor apparatus. Imamizu and Shimojo (1995) have theorized that transfer of a skill takes place at the task (cognitive) level rather than at the effector (hand) level [9]. Teixeira and Caminha (2003) tested this hypothesis in three groups of young participants [10]. Twenty-nine participants were pseudorandomly assigned to one of three groups: symmetric force, asymmetric force, and control group. The participants in the asymmetric group performed strength training with a dumbbell for nine weeks on their dominant (right) hand. Thus after nine weeks they had unequal strength between their dominant and non-dominant hand. The participants in the symmetric group received no strength training and thus they were assumed to have equal strength in both hands. Both asymmetric and symmetric group practiced a novel learning task all of pushing a small cart across a metallic track way by flexing the dominant wrist, striving to achieve a velocity of 70 cm/s. A third control group did not participate in the novel learning task. Results revealed that the symmetrical group attained a higher transfer of learning compared to the asymmetric and control groups when directional trend of error was used as the outcome measure. The asymmetrical group had a significant target overshoot compared to the symmetric group. These findings suggested that force characteristics are learned at the cognitive (task) level in the brain and there is an active interaction between the brain and the hand (manipulator) in achieving the task goal [10]. Brinkman and Kuypers (1973) suggested that transfer advantage might be different when the brain learns a task using the proximal muscle system [11]. It is known that the contralateral cerebral hemisphere controls distal movements while both the contralateral and ipsilateral hemispheres are involved in proximal movements [12]. When the main movements in a motor act are controlled with the proximal muscles versus the distal muscles a transfer advantage of learning is to be expected because both cerebral hemispheres are activated in the original motor learning task. One can also theoretically argue that certain aspects of the task might promote the transfer advantage of a fine motor skill if it is first acquired using the proximal muscles. It is known that the cortical motor representations of the shoulder and elbow muscles are smaller than the hand muscles [12]. Fewer muscles in the proximal motor apparatus allow fewer degrees of freedom compared to the many degrees of freedom in the hand motor apparatus. When learning a new skill with the fine motor apparatus more muscles are used to manipulate an object, and requiring the brain to learn the pattern entailed by the muscles, initiating a complex motor learning. According to Rose (1997), during the advance stage of learning the skill, the learner begins to re-establish or release additional degrees of freedom, adding variability to the skill [13]. This release of additional degrees of freedom allows the joint and associated muscles to move more independent of each other as the performance of the skill becomes more fluent and adaptable to a changing environment. Since the hand has more degrees of freedom (approximately twenty) compared to the proximal arm (approximately five) one could speculate that when learning a new skill with the proximal arm, the learner has fewer degrees of freedom to control. This condition might prove beneficial at least in the acquisition stage of motor learning. A proximal to distal transfer advantage of a motor skill might have important occupational therapy implication, for example, whether handwriting is better learned using the proximal muscles first in children. On a practical side, whether a child with poor handwriting skills would have an advantage if he/she practices a writing skill on the chalkboard first which requires the use of the proximal muscle group and then transfers the skill on paper, which requires the distal muscle group. The purpose of the study was to investigate whether typically developing children learn a motor skill better if the skill is first learned by the proximal motor apparatus and then transferred to the distal motor apparatus. It is important to learn a new motor skill through repeated practice. It is also equally important to retain the skill and reproduce it on a future recall. Thus it would be important to investigate whether transfer advantage of a skill remains after a period of time. Using a 2x2 repeated measure mixed ANOVA design (one between subject factor with two directions and one within subject factor with three levels) the following hypotheses were formulated for the present study, in a pattern-drawing task, in typical developing children between 5–13 years old:
| ||||||
Materials and Methods
| ||||||
Participants Apparatus Procedure For the children in the proximal group, the dry erase board with the larger pattern containing the numbers for direction was placed in front of the child in midline. The child's non-dominant hand was placed on his/her lap while the dominant hand was placed on the dry erase board at the starting point of the drawing pattern. The researcher verbally told the child to slide the writing stylus connecting the dots in the order from one to five using the power grasp. Once the child practiced the drawing with the feedback (Practice Learning or PL) and understood the task, the child used the opposite end of the writing stylus (inkless side to avoid feedback) to repeat the pattern for a maximum of 30 trials to learn to draw the pattern correctly following the visual cues of numbers. The researcher then erased the pattern and used the transparency to draw the pattern without the visual cues. After a gap of approximately five minutes, the child was asked to reproduce the pattern twice using the power grasp (OL). After another approximately 5-minute period, the child was asked to draw a smaller version of the pattern twice using a tripod grasp (TL). After a 24-hour period, the researcher went back the child's home and asked the child to produce the pattern twice in the same condition of transfer learning. This trial is called delayed learning. For the distal group, exactly the opposite protocol was followed in terms of grasp as detailed in the flow chart (Figure 2). A maximum of five verbal prompts were provided throughout the experiment if the child required additional help (Figure 3). Study design and data analysis | ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
Results | ||||||
The means and standard deviations for the drawing time and total errors by the directions are presented in Table 1. It can be seen that the mean time taken to produce the drawing by proximal apparatus (OL) is longer than the time taken by the distal apparatus both in TL and in DL. On the other hand, mean time taken to produce the drawing by distal apparatus (OL) is less than the time taken by proximal apparatus in both TL and DL Figure 5. The results of the ANOVA for draw time and drawing errors are presented in Table 2. The results indicated that the main factor of directions and main factor of conditions (3 levels of learning) yielded no significance. However, the interaction between the main factor of direction (between subject factor) and condition (within subject factor of learning conditions) yielded significance with low effect size. Contrast analysis revealed a significant difference in this interaction effect between original learning and transfer learning (F (1, 28) = 9.421, p=0.005). Table 1 shows that participants made fewer errors when they practiced with the distal motor apparatus in contrast to practicing with the proximal motor apparatus. The results of total errors are also presented in Table 2. The results indicated that the main effects of direction yielded significance with large effect size. Thus, the differences in error between proximal original learning and distal original learning conditions were significant. However, the main effects of learning yielded no significance. In both cases, the skill learning was transferred and retained although no transfer advantage was indicated. The interaction between the two main factors failed to reach significance on any dependent variables. | ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
| ||||||
Discussion
| ||||||
Results revealed that skill was transferred when children practiced either with the proximal apparatus first or with the distal apparatus first. However, transfer advantage of the skill was not found in any direction. The results supported the hypothesis that there will be no significant differences in the dependent variables in transferring skill from one limb part to another part of the same limb. These findings are consistent with earlier studies [1] [2] in demonstrating an intramanual skill transfer from proximal to distal apparatus. Similarly, the results also supported the findings of Weigelt et al. (2000) who found a distal to proximal transfer of skill in a soccer juggling skill with the lower extremities [3]. It is intriguing to note that the children made fewer errors when drawing first with distal motor apparatus compared to proximal apparatus. In addition, drawing errors did not increase or decrease with transfer. In other words, even though the distal apparatus made in general fewer errors the overall number of errors (transfer of skill) was consistent regardless of what apparatus was utilized first. For example, if the participant made n-number of errors when first using the proximal apparatus then he/she made equal errors when the skill was transferred to the distal apparatus. The same phenomenon occurred during the transfer of skill using the distal apparatus first. If the participant made n-number errors when using the distal apparatus first then the participant made equal errors when transferring the skill to the proximal apparatus. The transfer of skill was consistent whether using the proximal or distal apparatus first, thus contradicting the first our hypothesis. In addition, it is possible that less total errors occurred at the distal apparatus because typically developing children, perhaps, have already learned to control the degrees of freedom at the distal apparatus. Therefore, the argument of using the distal apparatus first may be true for typically developing children. Although not significant, the time difference between the three levels of learning could be due to size effect. In actuality the size of the drawing should be a cofactor because the proximal motor apparatus drew a bigger drawing and the distal apparatus drew a smaller drawing. Thus, the timing decline during P2D and timing increase during D2P are probably a drawing size effect rather than a learning effect. This argument is emboldened by the argument that there is no change in drawing errors in one particular group. Thus, not controlling the size of the drawing as a co variable is a limitation in analysis. In summary, this initial exploratory study indicated that children would be able to learn a novel motor task using either their proximal motor apparatus that is shoulder and elbow or using their distal motor apparatus that is wrist and hands. Additionally, such learning can be transferred distally or proximally. Although the results of the study are based on typically developing children, further research should be conducted to include children with disabilities. Occupational therapists often work with children who have mental retardation and developmental disorders (e.g., intellectual disability) who frequently display problems with fine motor skills such as handwriting, tying shoes, manipulation small objects, eating, and managing zippers [4]. Intellectual disability/mental retardation is a disorder that is characterized by a significantly below average intelligence and deficits in two or more skill areas, which include occupations of daily living, communication, social, academic, leisure, and homemaking [5]. Children with intellectual disability have deficits in cognitive development such as slower learning ability, shorter attention span, difficulty with problem solving and critical thinking, difficulty generalizing information and mastering abstract thinking, and difficulty storing and retrieving information [6]. Along with cognitive deficits, children with intellectual disability also display deficits in motor development. According to Schmidt and Lee (2005), one problem in general with motor programming is lack of storage capacity [7]. Therefore, future research should focus on determining if it would be advantageous for children with intellectual disability to learn drawing tasks, writing, and other skills by using the proximal apparatus first. This could have important occupational therapy implications. For example working on writing using the chalkboard first using the proximal apparatus might benefit the intellectual disability population to then transfer the skill to the distal apparatus. Through these skills transfer, children with intellectual disability could get more opportunities to participate school-based activity and decrease burden of teachers who teach school functions in a class. Limitations | ||||||
Conclusion
| ||||||
Children with intellectual disability have difficulty with storing information, therefore making new motor patterns difficult to learn. To take the advantage of the principle of motor learning transfer, proximal training of a fine motor task might be advantageous in the intellectual disability population for two reasons: a) it would be a cognitive advantage to use bilateral brain maps as in proximal muscle use and b) it would also be beneficial to start with smaller degrees of freedom to better control the skill learning. It would be useful to examine whether this finding is true for children with fine motor problems. The issue to ponder is whether children with fine motor delay would benefit from using the proximal apparatus first because fewer degrees of freedom are used. Thus, further research to examine this issue is needed. | ||||||
| ||||||
Acknowledgements
| ||||||
We would like to thank occupational therapy students in University of Toledo for collecting data to conduct this study. | ||||||
References
| ||||||
|
[HTML Abstract]
[PDF Full Text]
|
Author Contributions:
Kinsuk Maitra – Substantial contributions to conception and design, Acquisition of data, Drafting the article, Revising it critically for important intellectual content; Final approval of the version to be published Martin S. Rice – Substantial contributions to conception and design, Analysis and interpretation of data, Drafting the article, Final approval of the version to be published Hae Yean Park – Substantial contributions to conception and design, Drafting the article, Revising it critically for important intellectual content, Final approval of the version to be published |
Guarantor of submission
The corresponding author is the guarantor of submission. |
Source of support
None |
Conflict of interest
Authors declare no conflict of interest. |
Copyright
© 2016 Kinsuk Maitra et al. This article is distributed under the terms of Creative Commons Attribution License which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided the original author(s) and original publisher are properly credited. Please see the copyright policy on the journal website for more information. |
|